¿Viviendo o muriendo? La influencia de las emociones provocadas por las palabras en los tiempos de reacción manual
DOI:
https://doi.org/10.15448/1980-8623.2024.1.42765Palabras clave:
Tarea de compatibilidad espacial afectiva, Valencia afectiva, Dinámica temporalResumen
Los estímulos afectivos influyen en el comportamiento debido a las facilitaciones/inhibiciones que se producen en el sistema sensoriomotor. Para los estímulos positivos, las respuestas ipsilaterales tienden a ser facilitadas y las contralaterales inhibidas. Para los estímulos negativos, el patrón se invierte. Actualmente, 34 voluntarios fueron sometidos a la Tarea de Compatibilidad Espacial Afectiva, cuyos estímulos de valencia innata fueron las palabras "vivir" y "morir". En el mapeo 1, se ejecutaron respuestas ipsilaterales para la palabra "vivir" y contralaterales para la palabra "morir". En el mapeo 2, ocurrió lo contrario. Mediante un análisis temporal, investigamos si las palabras que desencadenan emociones innatas modulan la respuesta motora y cómo lo hacen. En el mapeo 1, encontramos respuestas ipsilaterales más lentas a la palabra "vivir" que respuestas contralaterales a la palabra "morir" (del tercer quintil). Sin embargo, el mapeo 2 reveló una diferencia sólo en el 3er quintil. Los efectos facilitadores de la respuesta contralateral al estímulo negativo están posiblemente asociados a mecanismos automáticos de vigilancia para detectar/evitar estímulos amenazantes.
Descargas
Citas
Alves, N. T., Fukusima, S. S., & Aznar-Casanova, J. A. (2008). Models of brain asymmetry in emotional processing. Psychology & Neuroscience, 1(1), 63-66. http://dx.doi.org/10.3922/j.psns.2008.1.010
Åhs, F., Rosén, J., Kastrati, G., Fredrikson, M., Agren, T., & Lundström, J. N. (2018). Biological preparedness and resistance to extinction of skin conductance responses conditioned to fear relevant animal pictures: A systematic review. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 95, 430-437. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.10.017
Arioli, M., Basso, G., Poggi, P., & Canessa, N. (2021). Fronto-temporal brain activity and connectivity track implicit attention to positive and negative social words in a novel socio-emotional Stroop task. NeuroImage, 226, 117580. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117580
Ashley, V., Honzel, N., Larsen, J., Justus, T., & Swick, D. (2013). Attentional bias for trauma-related words: Exaggerated emotional Stroop effect in Afghanistan and Iraq war veterans with PTSD. BMC Psychiatry, 13(1), 86. https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-244X-13-86
Azaad, S., Laham, S. M., & Shields, P. (2019). A meta-analysis of the object-based compatibility effect. Cognition, 190, 105–127. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2019.04.028
Beatty, G. F., & Janelle, C. M. (2020). Emotion regulation and motor performance: An integrated review and proposal of the Temporal Influence Model of Emotion Regulation (TIMER). International Review of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 13(1), 266-296. https://doi.org/10.1080/1750984X.2019.1695140
Bebbington, K., MacLeod, C., Ellison, T. M., & Fay, N. (2017). The sky is falling: Evidence of a negativity bias in the social transmission of information. Evolution and Human Behavior, 38(1), 92–101. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2016.07.004
Berne, R. M., & Levy, M. N. (Eds.) (2018). Fisiologia (7. ed.). Guanabara Koogan.
Blakemore, R. L., & Vuilleumier, P. (2017). An Emotional Call to Action: Integrating Affective Neuroscience in Models of Motor Control. Emotion Review, 9(4), 299-309. https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073916670020
Brosch, T., Pourtois, G., Sander, D., & Vuilleumier, P. (2011). Additive effects of emotional, endogenous, and exogenous attention: Behavioral and electrophysiological evidence. Neuropsychologia, 49(7), 1779–1787. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.02.056
Brosch, T., Scherer, K. R., Grandjean, D., & Sander, D. (2013). The impact of emotion on perception, attention, memory, and decision-making. Swiss Medical Weekly, 143, w13786. https://doi.org/10.4414/smw.2013.13786
Burke, R. E. (2007). Sir Charles Sherrington’s The integrative action of the nervous system: A centenary appreciation. Brain, 130(4), 887-894. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awm022
Carlson, J. M., Conger, S., & Sterr, J. (2018). Auditory Distress Signals Potentiate Attentional Bias to Fearful Faces: Evidence for Multimodal Facilitation of Spatial Attention by Emotion. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 42(4), 417-426. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-018-0282-7
Cavallet, M., Chaim-Avancini, T. M., Biazoli, C. E., Bazán, P. R., da Silva, M. A., Cunha, P. J., Miguel, C. S., Busatto, G. F., Louzã, M. R., & Gawryszewski, L. G. (2016). Influence of emotional stimulus valence on inhibitory control in adults with and without ADHD. Experimental Brain Research, 234(11), 3213-3223. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-016-4719-0
Chen, M., & Bargh, J. A. (1999). Consequences of Automatic Evaluation: Immediate Behavioral Predispositions to Approach or Avoid the Stimulus. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25(2), 215-224. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167299025002007
Conde, E. F. Q., Lucena, A. O. de S., da Silva, R. M., Filgueiras, A., Lameira, A. P., Torro-Alves, N., Gawryszewski, L. G., & Machado-Pinheiro, W. (2018). Hemispheric specialization for processing soccer related pictures: The role of supporter fanaticism. Psychology & Neuroscience, 11(4), 329-341. https://doi.org/10.1037/pne0000147
Conde, E. Q., Cavallet, M., Torro-Alves, N., Matsushima, E. H., Fraga-Filho, R. S., Jazenko, F., Busatto, G., & Gawryszewski, L. G. (2014a). Effects of affective valence on a mixed Spatial Correspondence Task: A reply to Proctor (2013). Psychology & Neuroscience, 7(2), 83-90. https://doi.org/10.3922/j.psns.2014.021
Conde, E. F. Q., Matsushima, E. H., Torro-Alves, N., Cavallet, M., Jazenko, F., Fraga Filho, R. S., & Gawryszewski, L. G. (2014b). Affective spatial compatibility task (AFFSCT): Theory and applications. Temas Em Psicologia, 22(3), 625-638. https://doi.org/10.9788/TP2014.3-08
Conde, E. F. Q., Jazenko, F., Fraga Filho, R. S., da Costa, D. H., Torro-Alves, N., Cavallet, M., & Gawryszewski, L. G. (2011). Stimulus affective valence reverses spatial compatibility effect. Psychology & Neuroscience, 4(1), 81-87. http://dx.doi.org/10.3922/j.psns.2011.1.010
De Jong, R., Liang, C. C., & Lauber, E. (1994). Conditional and unconditional automaticity: A dual-process model of effects of spatial stimulus-response correspondence. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance, 20(4), 731-750. https://doi.org/10.1037//0096-1523.20.4.731
Doradzińska, Ł., Wójcik, M. J., Paź, M., Nowicka, M. M., Nowicka, A., & Bola, M. (2020). Unconscious perception of one’s own name modulates amplitude of the P3B ERP component. Neuropsychologia, 147, 107564. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2020.107564
Ekman, P. (1992). An argument for basic emotions. Cognition and Emotion, 6(3–4), 169-200. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699939208411068
Estes, Z., & Verges, M. (2008). Freeze or flee? Negative stimuli elicit selective responding. Cognition, 108(2), 557-565. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2008.03.003
Fazio, R. H. (2001). On the automatic activation of associated evaluations: An overview. Cognition and Emotion, 15(2), 115–141. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930125908
Ferri, F., Tajadura-Jiménez, A., Väljamäe, A., Vastano, R., & Costantini, M. (2015). Emotion-inducing approaching sounds shape the boundaries of multisensory peripersonal space. Neuropsychologia, 70, 468-475. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.03.001
Flykt, A. (2006). Preparedness for action: Responding to the snake in the grass. The American Journal of Psychology, 119(1), 29-43.
Fraga-Filho, R. S., Lameira, A. P., Rangel, M. L., de Oliveira, F. S., Jazenko, F., & Gawryszewski, L. G. (2018). Modulações dos efeitos Simon e Stroop espacial por tarefas prévias de compatibilidade espacial. [Modulations of Simon and spatial Stroop effects due to previous spatial compatibility tasks.]. PSICO, 49(3), 242-248. https://doi.org/10.15448/1980-8623.2018.3.26812
Gauer, G., Fontenele-Aráujo, J., & Landeira-Fernandez, J. (2019). Abordagem Psicobiológica em Psicologia e Neurociências. Editora Cognitiva.
Heyes, C., & Catmur, C. (2022). What Happened to Mirror Neurons? Perspectives on Psychological Science, 17(1), 153-168. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691621990638
Holas, P., Krejtz, I., Rusanowska, M., Rohnka, N., & Nezlek, J. B. (2019). Attention to negative words predicts daily rumination among people with clinical depression: Evidence from an eye tracking and daily diary study. Cognition & Emotion, 33(6), 1277–1283. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2018.1541168
Hall, J. E., & Guyton, A. C. (2017). Guyton & Hall tratado de fisiologia médica (13. ed.). Elsevier.
Huckfeldt, R., Levine, J., Morgan, W., & Sprague, J. (1999). Accessibility and the Political Utility of Partisan and Ideological Orientations. American Journal of Political Science, 43(3), 888-911. https://doi.org/10.2307/2991839
Imbir, K. K., Pastwa, M., Jankowska, M., Kosman, M., Modzelewska, A., & Wielgopolan, A. (2020). Valence and arousal of words in visual and conceptual interference control efficiency. PLOS ONE, 15(11), e0241694. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0241694
Joseph, D. L., Chan, M. Y., Heintzelman, S. J., Tay, L., Diener, E., & Scotney, V. S. (2020). The manipulation of affect: A meta-analysis of affect induction procedures. Psychological Bulletin, 146(4), 355-375. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000224
Kotlewska, I., & Nowicka, A. (2015). Present self, past self and close-other: Event-related potential study of face and name detection. Biological Psychology, 110, 201-211. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2015.07.015
Krieglmeyer, R., & Deutsch, R. (2013). Approach Does Not Equal Approach: Angry Facial Expressions Evoke Approach Only When It Serves Aggression. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 4(5), 607-614. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550612471060
Lameira, A. P., Pereira, A., Fraga-Filho, R. S., & Gawryszewski, L. G. (2015). Stimulus-response compatibility with body parts: A study with hands. Experimental Brain Research, 233(7), 2127-2132. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-015-4283-z
LeDoux, J. (2003). The emotional brain, fear, and the amygdala. Cellular and Molecular Neurobiology, 23(4–5), 727-738. https://doi.org/10.1023/a:1025048802629
Leys, C., Ley, C., Klein, O., Bernard, P., & Licata, L. (2013). Detecting outliers: Do not use standard deviation around the mean, use absolute deviation around the median. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 49(4), 764-766. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2013.03.013
Lucas, I., Sánchez‐Adam, A., Vila, J., & Guerra, P. (2019). Positive emotional reactions to loved names. Psychophysiology, 56(7), e13363. https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.13363
Lu, Y., Jaquess, K. J., Hatfield, B. D., Zhou, C., & Li, H. (2017). Valence and arousal of emotional stimuli impact cognitive-motor performance in an oddball task. Biological Psychology, 125, 105-114. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2017.02.010
Moors, A., Ellsworth, P. C., Scherer, K., & Frijda, N. (2013). Appraisal theories of emotion: state of the art and future development. Emotion Review, 5(2), 119-124.
Nascimento, P. F. D. do, Lameira, A. P., Torro, N., & Gawryszewski, L. G. (2020). Affective valence, spatial compatibility, and presidential candidates: A study on political rivalry in Brazilian elections. Psychology & Neuroscience, 13(2), 187-195. https://doi.org/10.1037/pne0000199
Nealis, L. J., Allen, Z. M. van, & Zelenski, J. M. (2016). Positive Affect and Cognitive Restoration: Investigating the Role of Valence and Arousal. PLOS ONE, 11(1), e0147275. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0147275
Nascimento, P. F. D., Alves, N. T., Fernandes, A. M., da Silva, J. A., de Gonzaga Gawryszewski, L., & Lameira, A. P. (2018). O efeito Simon induzido pela direção do olhar em faces de dor e alegria. [The Simon effect triggered by the gaze direction in painful and happy faces.]. PSICO, 49(1), 43-49. https://doi.org/10.15448/1980-8623.2018.1.26876
Neumann, R., Förster, J., & Strack, F. (2003). Motor compatibility: The bidirectional link between behavior and evaluation. In J. Musch & K. C. Klauer (Eds.), The psychology of evaluation: Affective processes in cognition and emotion (pp. 371-391). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.
Ohman, A., & Mineka, S. (2001). Fears, phobias, and preparedness: Toward an evolved module of fear and fear learning. Psychological Review, 108(3), 483-522. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295x.108.3.483
Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia, 9(1), 97-113. https://doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4
Pool, E., Brosch, T., Delplanque, S., & Sander, D. (2016). Attentional bias for positive emotional stimuli: A meta-analytic investigation. Psychological Bulletin, 142(1), 79-106. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000026
Ratcliff, R. (1979). Group reaction time distributions and an analysis of distribution statistics. Psychological Bulletin, 86(3), 446-461. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.86.3.446
Ridderinkhof, K. R. (2002). Micro- and macro-adjustments of task set: Activation and suppression in conflict tasks. Psychological Research, 66(4), 312-323. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-002-0104-7
Robinson, M. D., & Fetterman, A. K. (2015). The Embodiment of Success and Failure as Forward versus Backward Movements. PLOS ONE, 10(2), e0117285. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0117285
Shaham, G., Mortillaro, M., & Aviezer, H. (2020). Automatic facial reactions to facial, body, and vocal expressions: A stimulus-response compatibility study. Psychophysiology, 57(12), e13684. https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.13684
Smith, N. K., Cacioppo, J. T., Larsen, J. T., & Chartrand, T. L. (2003). May I have your attention, please: Electrocortical responses to positive and negative stimuli. Neuropsychologia, 41(2), 171-183. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0028-3932(02)00147-1
Tacikowski, P., Brechmann, A., & Nowicka, A. (2013). Cross-modal pattern of brain activations associated with the processing of self- and significant other’s name. Human Brain Mapping, 34(9), 2069-2077. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.22048
Tacikowski, P., Cygan, H. B., & Nowicka, A. (2014). Neural correlates of own and close-other’s name recognition: ERP evidence. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 194. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00194
Tsal, Y., & Lavie, N. (1993). Location dominance in attending to color and shape. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance, 19(1), 131-139. https://doi.org/10.1037//0096-1523.19.1.131
Todd, J., van Ryckeghem, D. M. L., Sharpe, L., & Crombez, G. (2018). Attentional bias to pain-related information: A meta-analysis of dot-probe studies. Health Psychology Review, 12(4), 419-436. https://doi.org/10.1080/17437199.2018.1521729
Umiltá, C., & Nicoletti, R. (1990). Spatial stimulus-response compatibility. In R. W. Proctor & T. G. Reeve (Eds.), Stimulus-response compatibility: An integrated perspective (Vol. 65, pp. 89-116). North-Holland.
Verhage, A., Noppe, J., Feys, Y., & Ledegen, E. (2018). Force, Stress, and Decision-Making Within the Belgian Police: The Impact of Stressful Situations on Police Decision-Making. Journal of Police and Criminal Psychology, 33(4), 345-357. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11896-018-9262-4
Yamaguchi, M., & Chen, J. (2019). Affective influences without approach-avoidance actions: On the congruence between valence and stimulus-response mappings. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 26(2), 545-551. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-018-1547-1
Yamaguchi, M., Chen, J., Mishler, S., & Proctor, R. W. (2018). Flowers and spiders in spatial stimulus-response compatibility: Does affective valence influence selection of task-sets or selection of responses? Cognition & Emotion, 32(5), 1003-1017. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2017.1381073
Zsido, A. N., Csatho, A., Matuz, A., Stecina, D., Arato, A., Inhof, O., & Darnai, G. (2019). Does Threat Have an Advantage After All? – Proposing a Novel Experimental Design to Investigate the Advantages of Threat-Relevant Cues in Visual Processing. Frontiers in Psychology, 10. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02217
Descargas
Publicado
Cómo citar
Número
Sección
Licencia
Derechos de autor 2024 Psico
Esta obra está bajo una licencia internacional Creative Commons Atribución 4.0.
Derechos de Autor
La sumisión de originales para la Psico implica la transferencia, por los autores, de los derechos de publicación. El copyright de los artículos de esta revista es el autor, junto con los derechos de la revista a la primera publicación. Los autores sólo podrán utilizar los mismos resultados en otras publicaciones indicando claramente a Psico como el medio de la publicación original.
Creative Commons License
Excepto donde especificado de modo diferente, se aplican a la materia publicada en este periódico los términos de una licencia Creative Commons Atribución 4.0 Internacional, que permite el uso irrestricto, la distribución y la reproducción en cualquier medio siempre y cuando la publicación original sea correctamente citada.